Ergebnis für URL: http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading4
   This is chapter 8 of the [1]"The Phenomenon of Science" by [2]Valentin F. Turchin
     ____________________________________________________________________________

   Contents:
     * [3]THE SYSTEM ASPECT OF CULTURE
     * [4]THE SAVAGE STATE AND CIVILIZATION
     * [5]THE METASYSTEM TRANSITION IN LINGUISTIC ACTIVITY
     * [6]THE MAGIC OF WORDS
     * [7]SPIRITS AND THE LIKE
     * [8]THE TRASH HEAP OF REPRESENTATIONS
     * [9]BELIEF AND KNOWLEDGE
     * [10]THE CONSERVATISM OF PRECRITICAL THINKING
     * [11]THE EMERGENCE OF CIVILIZATION
     ____________________________________________________________________________

                                      CHAPTER EIGHT.
                                    PRIMITIVE THINKING

THE SYSTEM ASPECT OF CULTURE

   LET US CONTINUE our excursion through the stages of evolution. The subject of our
   analysis now will be the history of the development of language and thinking, the
   most important component of ''spiritual" culture. As we have already noted, the
   division of culture into "material" and ''spiritual'' is quite arbitrary and the
   terms themselves do not reflect the substance of the division very accurately, so
   that when we want to emphasize this we place them within quotation marks. The use
   of a tool and, even more so, the creation of new ones demand the work of
   imagination and are accompanied by emotions, giving us grounds to consider these
   phenomena part of ''spiritual'' culture. At the same time, the process of
   thinking manifests itself as definite linguistic activity directed to completely
   material objects-- linguistic objects. Language and thinking are very closely
   interconnected with material culture. The historian who sets himself the task of
   investigating the mechanism of the development of culture can only consider these
   phenomena in their interrelationship. He must also take account of other aspects
   of culture--above all the social structure of society--as well as the influence
   of natural conditions, historical accidents, and other factors. But the present
   investigation is not historical. Our task is simpler: without going into the
   details of historical development to describe what happened from a cybernetic or,
   as is also said, from a systems point of view. As with the question of the origin
   of human beings, we shall not be interested in a profound, intricate presentation
   of the historical circumstances that led to the particular step in the
   development of culture at the particular place and time. Our approach remains
   very global and general. We are interested in just one aspect of culture (but it
   is the most important one in the mechanics of development!)--its structure as a
   control hierarchy. Accordingly, we will view the development of culture also as a
   process of increasing complexity in this hierarchy through successive metasystem
   transitions. We will show, as was also true in the case of biological
   development, that the most important stages in the development of language and
   thinking are separated from one another by precisely these metasystem
   transitions.

THE SAVAGE STATE AND CIVILIZATION

   IN THE DEVELOPMENT of culture we discern above all two clearly distinct steps:
   the savage state (primitive culture) and civilization. The clear delineation
   between them does not mean that there are no transitional forms at all; the
   transition from the savage state to civilization is not carried out
   instantaneously, of course. But once it has begun, the development of culture
   through the creation of civilization takes place so rapidly that an obvious and
   indisputable difference between the new level of culture and the old manifests
   itself in a period of time which is vastly smaller than the time of existence in
   the savage state. The emergence of civilization is a qualitative leap forward.
   The total time of existence of civilization on Earth (not more than 5,000 to
   6,000 years) constitutes a small part of the time (at least 40,000 years) during
   which the human race has existed as a biologically invariable species. Thus, the
   emergence of civilization is a phenomenon which belongs entirely to the sphere of
   culture and is in not linked to the biological refinement of the human being.
   This distinguishes it from the emergence of language and labor activity but the
   consequences of this phenomenon for the biosphere are truly enormous, even if
   they are measured by simple quantitative indexes rather than by the complexity of
   the structures which emerge. In the short time during which civilization has
   existed, the human race has had incomparably more effect on the face of the
   planet than during the many millennia of the savage state. The size of the human
   race and its effect on the biosphere have grown at a particularly swift pace in
   the last three centuries; this is a result of the advances of science, the
   favorite child of civilization.

   This fact requires explanation. Such an abrupt qualitative leap forward in the
   observed manifestations of culture must be linked to some essential, fundamental
   change in the internal structure of culture. Language is the core of culture; it
   insures its uniformity, its "nervous system". We have in mind here not language
   as an abstract system possessing particular grammatical characteristics and used
   for expressing thoughts, but rather language as a living reality, as the social
   norm of linguistic activity. In other words, we have in mind the full observed
   (material if you like) side of thinking. Therefore, when we say ''language" we
   immediately add "and thinking". So language (and thinking) are the nervous system
   of culture and it may therefore be expected that there is some important
   difference between the language and thinking of primitive and of modern peoples.
   Indeed, a study of the culture of backward peoples reveals that they have a way
   of thinking which greatly differs from that of modern Europeans. This difference
   is by no means simply one in levels of knowledge. If a European is placed under
   primitive conditions he will hardly be able to use (or even show!) his knowledge
   of Ohm's law, the chemical formula for water, or the fact that the Earth revolves
   around the Sun and not vice versa. But the difference in way of thinking, in the
   approach to the phenomena of reality, remains and will quickly show itself in
   behavior.

   That difference can be summarized as follows. To a primitive person the observed
   phenomena of the world appear to be caused by invisible, supernatural beings. The
   primitives resort to incantations, ritual dances, sacrifices, strictly observed
   prohibitions (tabus), and so on to appease or drive off such beings. E. Taylor,
   one of the founders of the scientific study of primitive cultures, has called
   this view of the world animism, assuming the existence of spirits in all objects.
   To primitive people, certain mysterious relations and influences can exist
   between different objects ("mystic participation,'' in the terminology of the
   French ethnographer L. Levy Bruhl). Such relations always exist, in particular
   between the object and its image, or name. From this follow primitive magic and
   belief in the mystical connection between the tribe and a particular animal
   species (the totem ).

   But what is most surprising to the European is not the content of the
   representations of primitive people, rather it is their extreme resistance and
   insensitivity to the data of experience. Primitive thinking is inconceivably
   conservative and closed. Obvious facts which, in the European's opinion, would
   inevitably have to change the notions, of the primitive individual and force him
   to reconsider certain convictions do not, for some reason, have any effect on him
   at all. And attempts to persuade and prove often lead to results diametrically
   opposite to what was expected. It is this, not the belief in the existence of
   spirits and a mystical connection among objects, which is the more profound
   difference between primitive and modern thinking. In the last analysis,
   everything in the world is truly interrelated! When presenting the law of
   universal gravity we could say that there is a spirit of gravity in every body
   and each spirit strives to draw closely to the other spirit with a force
   proportional to the mass of the two bodies and inversely proportional to the
   square of the distance between them. This would not hinder us at all in correctly
   calculating the movement of the planets. But even if we do not use the word
   ''spirit,'' we still use the word ''force.'' And, in actuality, what is the
   Newtonian force of gravity? It is the same spirit: something unseen, unheard,
   unfelt, without taste or smell, but nonetheless really existing and influencing
   things.

   These characteristics of the thinking of primitive people are amazingly
   widespread. It can be said that they are common to all primitive peoples,
   regardless of their racial affiliation and geographic conditions and despite
   differences in the concrete forms of culture where they manifest themselves. This
   gives us grounds to speak of primitive thinking, juxtaposing it to modern
   thinking and viewing it as the first, historically inevitable phase of human
   thinking. Without negating the correctness of such a division or of our attempts
   to explain the transition, it should be noted that, as with any division of a
   continuous process into distinct phases, there are transitional forms too; in the
   thinking of a modern civilized person we often discern characteristics that go
   back to the intellectual activity of mammoth and cave-bear hunters.

THE METASYSTEM TRANSITION IN LINGUISTIC ACTIVITY

   THE PRIMITIVE PHASE is the phase of thinking which follows immediately after the
   emergence of language and is characterized by the fact that linguistic activity
   has not yet become its own object. The transition to the phase of modern thinking
   is a metasystem transition, in which there is an emergence of linguistic activity
   directed to linguistic activity. The language of primitive people is first-level
   language, while the language of modern people is second-level language (which
   specifically includes grammar and logic). But the transition to modern thinking
   is not simply a metasystem transition in language if we view language statically,
   as a certain possibility or method of activity. It includes a metasystem
   transition in real linguistic activity as a socially significant norm of
   behavior. With the transition to the phase of modern thinking it is not enough to
   think about something: one must also ask why one thinks that way, whether there
   is an alternative line of thought, and what would be the consequences of these
   particular thoughts. Thus, modern thinking is critical thinking, while primitive
   thinking can be called precritical. Critical thinking has become so accepted that
   it is taken for granted today. It is true that we sometimes say that a particular
   individual thinks uncritically: however, the term itself means that uncritical
   thinking is the exception, not the rule. An uncritical quality in thinking is
   ordinarily considered a weakness, and attempts are made to explain it in some
   way-- perhaps by the influence of emotions, a desire to avoid certain
   conclusions, and so on. In the case of certain convictions (dogmas. for example),
   uncritical thinking may be justified by their special (or sacred) origin. But the
   general stream of our thinking continues to be critical. This does not mean that
   it is always original and free of stereotypes, but even when we think in
   stereotyped ways we are nonetheless thinking critically because of the nature of
   the stereotype. It includes linguistic activity directed to linguistic activity,
   it teaches to separate the name from the meaning and remember the arbitrary
   nature of the connection between them, and it teaches us to think. ''Why do I
   talk or think this way?'' Not only do we use this stereotype, we also employ the
   results of its use by preceding generations.

   Things are different in primitive society, where the relation between language
   and reality is not yet the object of thought. There the social norm of thinking
   is to treat the words, notions, and rules of one's culture as something
   unconditionally given, absolute, and inseparable from other elements of reality.
   This is a very fundamental difference from the modern way of thinking. Let us
   consider primitive thinking in more detail and show that its basic observed
   characteristics follow from this feature, its precritical nature.

   We use below material from the writings of L. Levy-Bruhl, Primitive
   Thinking.[[12]1] This book combines material from Levy-Bruhl's La mentalité
   primitive and Les fonctions mentales dans les sociétés inférieures). This book is
   interesting because it collects a great deal of material on primitive culture
   which convincingly demonstrates the difference between primitive and modern
   thinking. A feature of Levy-Bruhl's conception is that he describes the thinking
   of individual members of primitive society as controlled by the collective
   representations of the given culture (actually, of course, this does not apply
   only to primitive society, but Levy-Bruhl somehow does not notice this). Also to
   Levy-Bruhl's credit is his observation that collective representations in
   primitive society differ fundamentally from our own and therefore it is
   completely incorrect to explain the thinking of a primitive person by assuming
   (often unconsciously) that he is modern. The rest of Levy-Bruhl's conception is
   quite unimportant. He describes primitive thinking as ''prelogical," "mystically
   oriented," and "controlled by the law of participation". These concepts remain
   very vague and add nothing to the material which has been collected. Only the
   term ''prelogical" thinking arouses our interest: it resembles our definition of
   primitive thinking as precritical.

THE MAGIC OF WORDS

   THE ASSOCIATION name-meaning L[i]-R[i] already exists in primitive thinking for
   language has become a firmly established part of life; but the association has
   not yet become an object of attention, because the metasystem transition to the
   second level of linguistic activity still has not taken place. Therefore the
   association L[i]-R[i] is perceived in exactly the same way as any association
   R[i]-R[j ]among elements of reality, for example the association between
   lightning and thunder. For primitive thinking the relation between an object and
   its name is an absolute (so to speak physical) reality which simply cannot be
   doubted. In fact--and this follows from the fundamental characteristic of the
   association--the primitive person thinks that there is a single object L[i]-R[i
   ]whose name L[i] and material appearance R[i] are different parts or aspects.
   Many investigators testify to the existence of this attitude toward names among
   primitive peoples. ''The Indian regards his name not as a mere label, but as a
   distinct part of his personality, just as much as are his eyes or his teeth, and
   believes that injury will result as surely from the malicious handling of his
   name as from a wound inflicted on any part of his physical organism. This belief
   was found among various tribes from the Atlantic to the Pacific.''[13][2]
   Therefore many peoples follow the custom of not using a person s ''real" name in
   everyday life, but instead using a nickname which is viewed as accidental and
   arbitrary. A. B. Ellis, who studied the peoples of West Africa, states that they
   '

   "believe that there is a real and material connection between a man and his name,
   and that by means of the name injury may be done to the man.... In consequence of
   this belief the name of the king of Dahomi is always kept secret.... It appears
   strange that the birth-name only, and not an alias, should be believed capable of
   carrying some of the personality of the bearer elsewhere . . . but the native
   view seems to be that the alias does not really belong to the man.''[14][3]

   This division of names into ''real'' and ''not real" is obviously the first step
   on the path toward the metasystem transition.

   The relation between an object and its image is perceived in exactly the same way
   as between an object and its name. In general primitive thinking does not make
   any essential distinction between the image and the name. This is not surprising,
   because the image is connected with the original of the same association that the
   name is. The image is the name and the name is the image. All images are names of
   an object taken together with the object itself form a single whole something
   (specifically a representation created by an association). Therefore it seems
   obvious that when we act on a part we act by the same token on the whole, which
   also means on its other parts By making an image of a buffalo pierced by an arrow
   the primitive believes that he is fostering a successful hunt for a real buffalo.
   G. Catlin, an artist and scientist who lived among the Mandans of North America,
   notes that they believed the pictures in the portraits he made borrowed a certain
   part of the life principle from their original. One of the Mandans told him that
   he knew he had put many buffalo in his book because the Indian was there while he
   drew them and after that observed that there were not so many buffalo for food.
   Obviously the Indian understood that the white man was not literally putting
   buffalo's in his book; but it was nevertheless obvious to him that in some sense
   (specifically in relation to the real-buffalo-buffalo-picture complexes) the
   white man was putting the buffalo in his book, because their numbers declined.
   The word "put" [the Russian ulozhit'--to put in, pack, fit] is used here in a
   somewhat metaphorical sense if the primary meaning refers to an action on a
   ''material" buffalo, but this does not affect the validity of the thought. Many
   terms in all the world's languages are used metaphorically, and without this the
   development of language would be impossible. When we use the Russian expression
   ulozhit' sya v golove [literally--to be packed, fit in the head; the idiomatic
   meaning is ''to be understood"] we do not mean that something has been put in our
   head in the same way that it is packed in a suitcase.

SPIRITS AND THE LIKE

   NOW LET US MOVE ON to "spirits," which play such an important part in primitive
   thinking. We shall see that the appearance of supernatural beings is an
   inevitable consequence of the emergence of language and that they disappear (with
   the same inevitability as they appeared) only upon the metasystem transition to
   the level of critical thinking.

   First let us think about the situation where language already exists but its
   relation to reality still has not become an object of study. Thanks to language,
   something like a doubling of objects occurs: instead of object R[i] a person
   deals with a complex R[i]L[i ]where L[i ]is the name of R[i] . In this complex,
   the linguistic object L[i] is the more accessible and, in this sense, more
   permanent component. One can say the word "sun" regardless of whether the sun is
   visible at the particular moment or not. One can repeat the name of a person as
   often as one likes while the person himself may be long dead. Each time his face
   will rise up in the imagination of the speaker. As a result the relation between
   the name and the meaning becomes inverted: the object L[i] acquires the
   characteristics of something primary and the object R[i] becomes secondary. The
   normal relation is restored only after the metasystem transition, when R[i] and
   L[i] are equally objects of attention, and the connection between them is of
   special importance. Until this has happened the word L[i] plays the leading role
   in the complex R[i]L[i ], and the faithful imagination is ready to link any
   pictures with each word used in social linguistic practices. Some words of the
   language of primitive culture signify objects which really exist from our modern
   point of view while others signify things which from our point of view do not
   really exist (spirits and so on). But from the point of view of the primitive
   individual there is no difference between them or perhaps simply a quantitative
   one. Ordinary objects may or may not be visible (perhaps they are hidden; perhaps
   it is dark). They may be visible only to some. The same is true of spirits, only
   it is harder to see them; either no one sees them or they are seen by sorcerers.
   Among the Klamath Indians in North America, the medicine man who was summoned to
   a sick person had to consult with the spirits of certain animals. Only one who
   had gone through a five-year course of preparation to be a medicine man could see
   these spirits, but he saw them just as plainly as the objects around himself. The
   Taragumars believed that large snakes with horns and enormous eyes lived in the
   rivers. But only shamans could see them. Among the Buryats the opinion was
   widespread that when a child became dangerously ill the cause was a little animal
   called an anokkha which was eating the top of the child s head away. The anokkha
   resembled a mole or cat, but only shamans could see it. Among the Guichols there
   is a ritual ceremony in which the heads of does are placed next to the heads of
   stags and it is considered that both the does and the stags have antlers,
   although no one except the shamans see them.

   There is an enormously broad variety of invisible objects in the representations
   of primitive peoples. They are not just formless spirits, but also objects or
   beings which have completely defined external appearances (except that they are
   not always perceived and not perceived by all). Language provides an abundance of
   material for the creation of imagined essences. Any quality is easily and without
   difficulty converted into an essence. The difference between a living person and
   a dead one produces the soul, and the difference between a sick person and a
   healthy one gives us illness. The representation of illness as something
   substantial, objective, which may enter and depart from a body and move in space,
   is perhaps typical of all primitive peoples. The same thing is true of the soul.
   It is curious that just as there are different illnesses among some peoples there
   also exist different "souls" in the human being. According to the observations of
   A. B. Ellis the Negroes of the West African coast distinguish two human spirits:
   kra and sraman. Kra lives in the person as long as he is alive but departs when
   the person sleeps; dreams are the adventures of the kra. When a person dies his
   kra may move to the body of another person or animal, but it may instead wander
   the world. The sraman forms only upon the death of the person and in the land of
   the dead continues the way of life which the deceased had followed.

   This belief shows even more clearly among the American Indians. The Maenads, for
   example, believe that every person carries several spirits: one of them is white,
   another is swarthy, and the third is a light color. The Dakotas believe that a
   person has four souls: the corporal soul, which dies along with the person; the
   spirit, which lives with the body or near it; the soul, responsible for the
   actions of the body; and the soul that always remains near a lock of the
   deceased's hair, which is preserved by relatives until it can be thrown onto
   enemy territory, whereupon it becomes a wandering ghost carrying illness and
   death. G. H. Jones, a scientist who studied beliefs in Korea, writes of spirits
   that occupy the sky and everywhere on earth. They supposedly lie in wait for a
   person along the roads, in the trees, in the mountains and valleys, and in the
   rivers and streams. They follow the person constantly even to his own home, where
   they have settled within the walls, hang from the beams, and attach themselves to
   the room dividers.

THE TRASH HEAP OF REPRESENTATIONS

   AS WE HAVE NOTED, it is not the fact of belief in the existence of invisible
   things and influences that distinguishes primitive thinking from modern thinking,
   but the content of the representations and particularly the relation between the
   content and the data of experience. We believe in the existence of neutrons
   although no one has ever seen them and never will. But we know that all the words
   in our vocabulary have meaning only to the extent that, taken together, they
   successfully describe observed phenomena and help to predict them. As soon as
   they stop fulfilling this role, as a result of new data from experience or owing
   to reorganisation of the system of word use (theory), we toss them aside without
   regret. That is what happened, for example, with "phlogiston'' or ether. Even
   earlier, all kinds of imagined beings and objects which were so typical of the
   thinking of our ancestors disappeared from language and thinking. What irritates
   us in primitive thinking is not the assumption of the existence of spirits but
   rather that this assumption, coming together with certain assumptions about the
   traits and habits of the spirits, explains nothing at all and often simply
   contradicts experience. We shall cite a few typical observations by
   investigators. In his Nicobar Island diaries, V. Solomon wrote: ''The people in
   all villages have performed the ceremony called "tanangla,'" signifying either
   "support" or "prevention". This is to prevent the illness caused by the
   north-east monsoon. Poor Nicobarese! They do the same thing year after year, but
   to no effect.'' [15][4]

   And M. Dobrizhoffer observed that

   A wound inflicted with a spear often gapes so wide that it affords ample room for
       life to go out and death to come in: yet if the man dies of the wound they
       madly believe him killed not by a weapon but by the deadly arts of the
       jugglers.... They are persuaded that the juggler will be banished from
       amongst the living and made to atone for their relation's death if the heart
       and tongue be pulled out of the dead man's body immediately after his
       decease, roasted at the fire and given to dogs to devour. Though so many
       hearts and tongues are devoured, and they never observed any of the jugglers
       die, yet they still religiously adhere to the custom of their ancestors by
       cutting out the hearts and tongues of infants and adults of both sexes, as
       soon as they have expired.[16][5]

   Because primitive people are unable to make their representations an object of
   analysis, these representations form a kind of trash heap. The trash heap
   accumulates easily but no one works to clean it up. For the primitive there are
   not and cannot be meaningless words. If he does not understand a word it
   frightens him as an unfamiliar animal, weapon, or natural phenomenon would. An
   opinion which has arisen as a result of the chance combination of circumstances
   is preserved from generation to generation without any real basis. The
   explanation of some phenomenon may be completely arbitrary and nonetheless fully
   satisfy the primitive. Critical thinking considers each explanation (linguistic
   model of reality) alongside other competing explanations (models) and it is not
   satisfied until it is shown that the particular explanation is better than its
   rivals. In logic this is called the law of sufficient grounds. The law of
   sufficient grounds is absolutely foreign to precritical thinking. It is here that
   the metasystem transition which separates modern thinking from primitive thinking
   is seen most clearly.

   Thanks to this characteristic the primitive's belief in the effectiveness of
   magic incantations, sorcery, and the like is unconquerable. His "theory" gives an
   explanation (often not just one but several!) for everything that happens around
   him. He cannot yet evaluate his theory--or even individual parts of
   it--critically. P. Bowdich tells of a savage who took up a fetish which was
   supposed to make him invulnerable. He decided to test it and let himself be shot
   in the arm; it broke his bone. The sorcerer explained that the offended fetish
   had just revealed to him the cause of what had happened: the young man had had
   sexual relations with his wife on a forbidden day. Everyone was satisfied. The
   wounded man admitted that it was true and his fellow tribesmen were only
   reinforced in their belief. Innumerable similar examples could be given.[17][6]

BELIEF AND KNOWLEDGE

   WHEN WE SAY that a primitive person believes in the existence of spirits or
   certain actions by them we predispose ourselves to an incorrect understanding of
   his psychology. When speaking of belief we juxtapose it to knowledge. But the
   very difference between belief and knowledge emerges only at the level of
   critical thinking and reflects a difference in the psychological validity of
   representations, which follows from the difference in their sources. For a
   primitive there is no difference between belief and knowledge and his attitude
   toward his representations resembles our attitude toward our knowledge, not our
   beliefs. From a psychological point of view the primitive person knows that
   spirits exist, he knows that incantations can drive out illness or inflict it,
   and he knows that after death he will live in the land of the dead. Therefore we
   shall avoid calling the primitive person's worldview primitive religion; the
   terms "primitive philosophy'' or "primitive science'' have equal right to exist.
   These forms of activity can only be distinguished at the level of critical
   thinking. This refers both to the difference between belief and knowledge and to
   the difference between the ''otherworldly'' and that which is ''of this world.''
   The fact that the representations of primitive people involve spirits, ghosts,
   shadows of the dead, and other devil figures still does not make these
   representations religious, because all of these things are perceived as entirely
   of this world and just as real (material if you like) as the animals, wind, or
   sunlight. L. Levy-Bruhl, who defines the psychological activity of primitive man
   as mystic, nonetheless emphasizes that this is not at all the same as mysticism
   in the modern meaning of the word. ''For lack of a better term,'' he writes, ''I
   am going to use this one; this is not because of its connection with the
   religious mysticism of our societies, which is something quite different, but
   because in the narrowest meaning of the word "mystic" is close to belief in
   forces, influences, and actions which are unnoticed and intangible to the senses
   but real all the same.'' Many observers are struck by how real the shadows or
   spirits of their ancestors seem to primitive peoples. R. Codrington writes about
   the Melanesians:[18][7] When a native says that he is a person, he wants it
   understood that he is a person not a spirit. He does not mean that he is a person
   not an animal. To him, intelligent beings in the world are divided into two
   categories: people who are alive and people who have died. In the Motu tribe this
   is ta-mur and ta-mate. When the Melanesians see white people for the first time
   they take them for ta-mate, that is, for spirits who have returned to life, and
   when the whites ask the natives who they are, the latter call themselves ta-mur,
   that is, people not spirits. Among the Chiriguanos of South America when two
   people meet they exchange this greeting: "Are you alive?''--"Yes, I am alive.''
   Some other South American tribes also use this form.

THE CONSERVATISM OF PRECRITICAL THINKING

   CONSERVATISM is inherent in precritical thinking; it is a direct consequence of
   the absence of an apparatus for changing linguistic models. All conceivable kinds
   of rules and prohibitions guide behavior and thinking along a strictly defined
   path sanctified by tradition. Violation of traditions evokes superstitious
   terror. There have been cases where people who accidentally violated a tabu died
   when they learned what they had done. They knew that they were supposed to die
   and they died as a result of self-suggestion.

   Of course, this does not mean that there is no progress whatsoever in primitive
   society. Within the limits of what is permitted by custom, primitive people
   sometimes demonstrate amazing feats of art, dexterity, patience, and persistence.
   Within the same framework tools and weapons are refined from generation to
   generation and experience is accumulated. The trouble is that these limitations
   are extremely narrow and rigid. Only exceptional circumstances can force a tribe
   (most likely the remnants of a tribe which has been destroyed by enemies or is
   dying from hunger) to violate custom. It was probably in precisely such
   situations that the major advances in primitive culture were made. A people which
   has fallen into isolation and owing to unfavorable natural conditions is not able
   to multiply and break up into bitterly hostile peoples may maintain its level of
   primitive culture unchanged for millennia.

   In the stage of precritical thinking, language plays a paradoxical role. In
   performance of its communicative function (communication among people, passing
   experience down from generation to generation, stablizing social groups) it is
   useful to people. But then its noncommunicative, modeling function causes more
   harm than good. This refers to those models which are created not at the level of
   the association of nonlinguistic representations but only at the level of
   language, that is, primarily the primitive "theory of spirits.'' As we have
   already noted, the communicative function itself becomes possible only thanks to
   the modeling function. But as long as linguistic models merely reflect neuronal
   models we speak of the purely communicative functions; when new models (theories)
   are created we speak of the noncommunicative function. In primitive society we
   see two theories: the rudiments of arithmetic (counting by means of fingers,
   chips, and the like) and the ''theory of spirits.'' Arithmetic is, of course, a
   positive phenomenon, but it does not play a major part in primitive life and is
   in fact absent among many peoples: the ''theory of spirits,'' on the contrary,
   permeates all primitive life and has a negative influence on it. And this is the
   paradox. The first independent steps of the linguistic system, which should
   according to the idea lead to (and later in fact do lead to) an enormous leap
   forward in modeling reality, at first produce poisonous discharges which retard
   further development. This is a result of the savage so to speak, growth of the
   ''theory of spirits.'' It can be compared with a weed which sprouts on
   well-fertilized soil if the garden is not managed. As we have seen, the weed's
   seeds are contained in the soil itself, in language. Only the transition to the
   level of critical thinking (careful cultivation of the soil, selection of plants
   for crops, and weed control) produces the expected yield.

THE EMERGENCE OF CIVILIZATION

   WE KNOW THAT this transition took place. The emergence of critical thinking was
   the most important milepost of evolution after the appearance of the human being.
   Critical thinking and civilization arise at the same time and develop in close
   interdependence. Increasing labor productivity, contacts among different tribal
   cultures, and the breakup of society into classes all inexorably weaken
   traditional tribal thinking and force people to reflect upon the content of their
   representations and compare them with those of other cultures. In this critical
   thinking takes root and gradually becomes the norm. On the other hand, critical
   thinking emancipates people and leads to a high rise in labor productivity and to
   the appearance of new forms of behavior. Both processes support and reinforce one
   another: society begins to develop swiftly. There is a kind of 180 degree turn in
   the vector of society's interest: in primitive society it is directed backward,
   to the past, to observance of the laws of ancestors; in a developing situation,
   at least among part of society (the "creative minority'' according to A.
   Toynbee), it is pointed forward, into the future, toward change in the existing
   situation. Thanks to a metasystem transition culture acquires dynamism and its
   own internal impetus toward development. The redirection of language activity to
   itself creates the stairway effect: each level of logical (language) thinking,
   which has emerged as a result of the analysis of logical thinking, becomes, in
   its turn an object of logical analysis. Critical thinking is an ultrametasystem
   capable of self-development. Primitive tribal cultures evolve by the formation of
   groups and the struggle for existence among them, just as in the animal world.
   Civilization evolves under the influence of internal factors. It is true that the
   civilizations of the past typically stopped in their development upon reaching a
   certain level; but all the same the leaps forward were extremely great in
   comparison with the advances of primitive cultures, and they grew larger as
   critical thinking became ever more established. Modern civilization is global, so
   that the factor of its struggle for existence as a whole (that is to say, against
   rivals) disappears and all its development occurs exclusively through the action
   of internal contradictions. Essentially, it was only with the transition to the
   level of critical thinking that the revolutionary essence of the emergence of
   thinking manifested itself, and the age of reason began in earnest.

   In the process of a metasystem transition there is, as we know, a moment when the
   new attribute demonstrates its superiority in a way which cannot be doubted, and
   from this moment the metasystem transition may be considered finally and
   irreversibly completed. In the transition to critical thinking this moment was
   the culture of Ancient Greece, which it is absolutely correct to call the cradle
   of modern civilization and culture. At that time, about 2.500 years ago,
   philosophy, logic, and mathematics (mathematics in the full sense of the word,
   that is to say, including proof) emerged. And from that time critical thinking
   became the recognized and essential basis of developing culture.
   _________________________________________________________________________________

   ^1 in Russian, Pervobytnoe myshlenie [Primitive Thinking]. Ateist Publishing
   House, 1930.

   [19][2] Original in James Mooney, "The Sacred Formulas of the Cherokee," 7th
   Annual Report of the Bureau of Ethnology. Washington, GPO, 1885-1886, p.
   343--trans.

   [20][3] Original A B. Ellis, The Ewe-Speaking People, London. 1890. p. 98--trans.

   [21][4] Original V. Solomon, "Extract from Diaries Kept in Car Nicobar," Journal
   of the Anthropological Institute of Great Brittain and Ireland 32 (January-June
   1902); trans .

   [22][5] Original in M Dobritzhoffer, An Account of the Abipones, (London, 1822),
   Vol 2, p. 223-trans.

   [23][6] T. Edward Bowdich, Mission from Cape Coast Castle to Ashantee, (London:
   Frank Cass and Co., Ltd, 3rd ed, 1966), p 439 - trans.

   [24][7] The Melanesian Languages (Oxford, 1891).
     ____________________________________________________________________________

References

   1. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/default.html
   2. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/turchin.html
   3. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading2
   4. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading3
   5. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading4
   6. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading5
   7. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading6
   8. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading7
   9. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading8
  10. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading9
  11. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#Heading10
  12. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#1
  13. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fn0
  14. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fn1
  15. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fn2
  16. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fn3
  17. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fn4
  18. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fn5
  19. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fnB0
  20. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fnB1
  21. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fnB2
  22. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fnB3
  23. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fnB4
  24. http://pespmc1.vub.ac.be/POS/Turchap8.html#fnB5


Usage: http://www.kk-software.de/kklynxview/get/URL
e.g. http://www.kk-software.de/kklynxview/get/http://www.kk-software.de
Errormessages are in German, sorry ;-)